Surprise, arousal, and pleasantness in movement between spaces

  • Margherita Brondino University of Verona
  • Jack Leon Nasar The Ohio State University https://orcid.org/0000-0002-8214-8257
  • Margherita Pasini University of Verona
  • Saleheh Bokharaei Shahid Beheshti University, Tehran

Abstract

In one theory as perceived surprise increases, arousal increases and pleasantness increase up to a point, after which it levels off or decreases. However, studies indicate that for environmental response, arousal and pleasantness are independent of one another. Those studies did not examine movement through spaces. We sought to study response to surprise as experienced in moving between pairs of offices. We created three simulated offices (A, B, and C) and nine virtual walks between each possible pair, such that some walks had no physical differences (AA, BB, and CC), some had moderate physical differences (AB, BA, BC, CB), and some had larger physical differences (AC, CA). A test confirmed that the manipulations worked as planned. To measure arousal and pleasantness, we created two three-item scales (each in English and Italian). We assigned participants in the US (121 adults, 47 men, 84 men) and Italy (67 adults, 343 men, 33 women) at random to either a within-group condition or one of the three between-group conditions (Low Surprise, Medium Surprise, or High Surprise). We used the within group to test the CFA model, and we used the between group conditions to test the effects of surprise. The CFA found the two three-item scales fit the multi-level model well. We combined the items into two three-item scales for the analysis of effects of surprise. Both arousal and pleasantness increased from low to moderate surprise, but decreased from moderate to high surprise. The results suggest value in studying dynamic environmental experience.

Author Biography

Jack Leon Nasar, The Ohio State University
Academy Professor, City & Regional Planning

References

Akalin, A., Yildirim, K., Wilson, C., & Kilicoglu, O. (2009). Architecture and engineering students' evaluations of house façades: Preference, complexity and impressiveness. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 29(1), 124-132. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2008.05.005

Berlyne, D. E. (1971). Aesthetics and psychobiology. New York: Appleton-Century.

Gaver, W. W., & Mandler, G. (1987). Play it again, Sam: On liking music. Cognition and Emotion, 1(3), 259-282. doi: 10.1080/02699938708408051

Heath, T., Smith, S. G., & Lim, B. (2000). Tall buildings and the urban skyline: The effect of visual complexity on preferences. Environment and behavior, 32(4), 541-556. doi: 10.1177/00139160021972658

Herzog, T. R., Herbert, E. J., Kaplan, R., & Crooks, C. L. (2000). Cultural and developmental comparisons of landscape perceptions and preferences. Environment and Behavior, 32(3), 323-346. doi: 10.1177/0013916500323002

Hunziker, M., Felber, P., Gehring, K., Buchecker, M., Bauer, N., & Kienast, F. (2008). Evaluation of landscape change by different social groups: results of two empirical studies in Switzerland. Mountain research and development, 28(2), 140-147. doi: 10.1659/mrd.0952

Huron, D. B. (2006). Sweet anticipation: Music and the psychology of expectation. MIT press.

King, C. C., Stokols, D., Talen, E., Brassington, G. S., Killingsworth, R. (2002). Theoretical approaches to the promotion of physical activity: Forging a transdisciplinary paradigm. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 23 (2S), 15-25. doi: 10.1016/S0749-3797(02)00470-1

Lozano, E. E. (1974). Visual needs in the urban environment. Town Planning Review, 45(4), 351. doi: 10.3828/tpr.45.4.h43m7270u0m3x968

Lyons, E. (1983). Environmental correlates of landscape preference. Environment and Behavior, 15, 487-511. doi: 10.1177/0013916583154005

Martindale, C., Moore, K., & Borkum, J. (1990). Aesthetic preference: Anomalous findings for Berlyne's psychobiological theory. The American Journal of Psychology, 53-80. doi: 10.2307/1423259

Nasar, J. L. (1987). The effect of sign complexity and coherence on the perceived quality of retail scenes. Journal of the American Planning Association, 53(4), 499-509. doi: 10.1080/01944368708977139

Nasar, J. L. (1994). Urban design aesthetics: The evaluative qualities of building exteriors. Environment and Behavior, 26, 377–401. doi: 10.1177/001391659402600305

Posner, J., Russell, J. A., & Peterson, B. S. (2005). The circumplex model of affect: An integrative approach to affective neuroscience, cognitive development, and psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 17, 715-734. doi:10.10170S0954579405050340

Russell, J. A. (1980). A circumplex model of affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 1161. doi: 10.1037/h0077714

Russell, J. A., Lewicka, M., & Nitt, T. (1989). A cross-cultural study of the circumplex model of affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 848-856. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.57.5.848

Russell, J. A., & Pratt, G. (1980). A description of the affective quality attributed to environments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38, 311. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.38.2.311

Solomon, R. L. (1980). The opponent-process theory of acquired motivation: the costs of pleasure and the benefits of pain. American psychologist, 35(8), 691.

Stamps III, A. E. (1999). Demographic effects in environmental aesthetics: A meta-analysis. Journal of Planning Literature, 14(2), 155-175. doi: 10.1177/08854129922092630

Van den Berg, A. E., Vlek, C. A., & Coeterier, J. F. (1998). Group differences in the aesthetic evaluation of nature development plans: a multilevel approach. Journal of environmental

psychology, 18(2), 141-157. doi: 10.1006/jevp.1998.0080

Wohlwill, J.F. (1968). Amount of stimulus exploration and preference as differential functions of stimulus complexity. Perception & Psychophysics, 4(5), 307-312. doi: 10.3758/BF03210521

Published
2019-06-20